Abstract
Evolutionarily relevant networks have been previously described in several mammalian species using time-averaged analyses of fMRI time-series. However, fMRI network activity is highly dynamic and continually evolves over timescales of seconds. Whether the dynamic organization of resting-state fMRI network activity is conserved across mammalian species remains unclear. Using frame-wise clustering of fMRI time-series, we find that intrinsic fMRI network dynamics in awake male macaques and humans is characterized by recurrent transitions between a set of 4 dominant, neuroanatomically homologous fMRI coactivation modes (C-modes), three of which are also plausibly represented in the male rodent brain. Importantly, in all species C-modes exhibit species-invariant dynamic features, including preferred occurrence at specific phases of fMRI global signal fluctuations, and a state transition structure compatible with infraslow coupled oscillator dynamics. Moreover, dominant C-mode occurrence reconstitutes the static organization of the fMRI connectome in all species, and is predictive of ranking of corresponding fMRI connectivity gradients. These results reveal a set of species-invariant principles underlying the dynamic organization of fMRI networks in mammalian species, and offer novel opportunities to relate fMRI network findings across the phylogenetic tree.
Subject terms: Functional magnetic resonance imaging, Computational neuroscience
Using dynamic fMRI time-series analysis in humans, macaques and mice, Gutierrez-Barragan et al. find that intrinsic brain network dynamics exhibits evolutionarily conserved, species-invariant features.
Introduction
Spontaneous fluctuations in resting fMRI signals have been consistently shown to be temporally synchronized across multiple functional systems, delineating a set of reproducible topographies often referred to as Resting-State Networks (RSNs)1,2. While RSN mapping typically entails the computation of time-averaged statistical dependencies between fMRI time-series to produce maps of static fMRI connectivity3, the last decade has brought forth a series of alternative time-varying approaches to investigate the temporal features of RSN activity4,5. Prompted by the need to complement human fMRI research with mechanistic investigations in physiologically accessible species6,7, multiple research groups have begun to implement fMRI mapping in non-human primates and rodents8,9. These investigations have revealed interesting evolutionary correspondences in the static organization of RSN across mammalian species. These encompass the presence of highly synchronous interhemispheric networks, including evolutionarily relevant precursors of distributed integrative systems, such as the default mode (DMN) and salience networks10–12. However, spontaneous brain activity is highly dynamic and continuously evolves over the timescale of minutes13,14. Accordingly, a large body of experimental and theoretical work has shown that the correlation structure of RSNs varies across time4,5 and involves transient interactions between distinct functional systems that are continually revisited15. These observations corroborate the notion that mere time-invariant descriptions of spontaneous fMRI activity are not sufficient to comprehensively describe the functional architecture of the resting brain.
Although many approaches to study the dynamic organization of RSNs have been proposed4,5, frame-wise methods16–19 have recently gained traction as a flexible approach to investigate the dynamic organization of intrinsic fMRI activity. Compared to correlation-based approaches (e.g., sliding-window analyses), frame-wise approaches offer the possibility to (a) temporally localize the peaks and troughs of activity that underlie fMRI network dynamics and relate them to global fluctuations in brain activity; (b) describe the dynamic organization of fMRI using physiologically interpretable parameters (e.g., mean fMRI activity); and (c) identify the relevant dimensions of fMRI dynamics without the need to pre-impose regional parcellations. Using whole-brain frame-wise clustering of fMRI time-series to obtain coactivation patterns (CAPs), it has been recently shown that intrinsic fMRI activity is dominated by recurring, cyclic fluctuations between stereotypic functional topographies16–19. The simplicity of the CAP framework, its direct association with a directly quantifiable physiological property, as well as its high temporal and spatial resolution are perfectly suited to parsimoniously, yet comprehensively describe the dynamic organization of spontaneous network activity.
While previous studies have compared the static organization of fMRI networks across species11,20, attempts to directly relate the dynamic organization of intrinsic brain activity across the mammalian phylogenetic tree are lacking. Is fMRI network dynamics underpinned by a unifying set of species-invariant principles, or does this phenomenon instead reflect unique, species-specific attributes? And are fMRI dynamic states and their cyclic dynamics evolutionarily conserved, or do they encompass phylogenetically divergent motifs?
To address these questions, we leveraged fMRI datasets acquired in awake humans, as well as male macaques and mice, to probe and compare the dynamic organization of fMRI signals in the mammalian brain. We find that fMRI network dynamics in all probed species are similarly characterized by cyclic transitions between a few dominant and neuroanatomically related fMRI Coactivation Modes (C-modes), which exhibit largely conserved topographies and dynamic features such as a quasi-periodic infra low evolution and structure of transitions between states compatible with a coupled-oscillators dynamics21. We further relate the occurrence of dynamic states represented by C-modes to the organization of the static connectome and fMRI connectivity gradients. These results suggest that resting fMRI activity in mammalian species is underpinned by evolutionarily conserved dynamic principles.
Results
Dominant coactivation modes parsimoniously describe fMRI network dynamics in humans, macaques, and mice
To compare the dynamic organization of fMRI network dynamics within an evolutionary perspective, we used a frame-wise approach based on the identification of coactivation patterns (CAPs)15,17. Data for this study consisted of awake rsfMRI datasets from two human cohorts (The Hangzhou Normal University - HNU: 30 subjects, 10 test-retest sessions22 and The Midnight Scan Club - MSC: 10 subjects, 5 test-retest sessions23); the Newcastle (NC) macaque cohort24,25 (8 animals with two test-retest sessions); and 44 mice under head-fixed conditions. Human data was sex balanced in both independent datasets, while all results for macaques and mice were obtained using male animals only. Using k-means, we clustered fMRI frames in the concatenated timeseries given their spatial similarity. We averaged the frames in each cluster to produce group-level CAP maps15–17, then T-scored them at the voxel level. We then obtained single-subject CAP maps by averaging the corresponding clustered frames from each subject (Fig. S1). To identify reproducible functional CAPs that are representative of the dynamic structure of fMRI networks in each species, we used a multi-criteria approach (see methods section) aimed at maximizing CAP reproducibility across individuals, sessions, or datasets. This approach yielded k = 8 CAPs in humans and macaques, and k = 6 in mice as optimal clustering solutions (see methods section and Figs. S2, S3). Previous work16 has shown that CAPs embody rich fMRI topographies that can be reliably matched into mirrored coactive and anti-coactive pairs characterized by opposite patterns of fMRI coactivation (Fig. 1). As we will demonstrate below, and similar to what has been observed in quasi-periodic patterns26, each matched CAP and anti-CAP pairs describe a cyclical fluctuation of a single fMRI state. As predicted, the employed procedure identified in all species mirrored CAP pairs characterized by opposite fMRI coactivation (spatial correlation, r < −0.65, all species, all CAP pairs, Fig. 1A, B). Importantly, the identified CAPs explained in all species a large proportion of variance in fMRI timeseries (R2 > 0.58 mice, >0.68 macaques, and >0.75 humans). Moreover, they also guaranteed robustness within and between dataset spatial and occurrence rate reproducibility (Figs. S2, S3, permutation tests with random CAP-identity shuffling). These results suggest that a few dominant dynamic patterns can parsimoniously describe the dynamic organization of fMRI activity in multiple mammalian species.
Fig. 1. C-mode identification and matching across species.
A CAPs represent transients of infraslow fMRI activity that can be matched in pairs exhibiting opposite coactivation (CAPs, anti-CAPs, left). Detection of CAP anti-CAP pairs (middle) allows for the computation of fMRI Coactivation modes (right). B Detection of CAP and anti-CAP pairs given the highest spatial anticorrelation in the between-CAP similarity matrix. C-modes are built by taking the highest occurring CAP from each pair, and spatially averaging it with its corresponding inverted anti-CAP. C Evolutionarily relevant fMRI networks used for matching. D Vectorized network coactivation profiles for each C-mode (spatially z-scored), extracted from the mean fMRI values of voxels within a network mask. Arrows denote the matching of C-modes across species performed by the Hungarian Algorithm. E Correlation between matched C-modes from humans (HNU dataset) and macaques, F between humans (MSC dataset) and macaques; and G between macaques and mice. aDMN anterior default mode, pDMN posterior default mode, SMN somatomotor, VIS visual, DAT dorsal attention, VAT ventral attention, FPN frontoparietal, LIMB limbic, TH thalamus, HC hippocampus. Source data are provided as a Source Data file.
To further reduce dimensionality and facilitate cross-species comparisons, we coalesced highly anticorrelated CAP pairs into a single Coactivation Mode (C-mode). CAP and anti-CAP are thus the peak and trough of the same fluctuating C-mode. C-modes are thus computed by reversing the sign of the anti-CAP, and spatially averaging it with its paired CAP such to spatially depict the corresponding coactivation axis (Fig. 1A). The topography of the individual CAPs pairs that constitute each C-mode is reported in Figure S4. To assess the potential confounding contribution of head motion to C-mode topography mapping, we repeated the clustering procedure on time-series in which we did not scrub fMRI frames exhibiting high motion-related. We next compared the spatial topography of C-modes obtained with and without frame censoring. This comparison revealed that the C-mode obtained using frame-censored timeseries were very similar to those we mapped using the entire timeseries (r > 0.95, all C-modes, all species, Fig. S5A). Further corroborating a negligible contribution of head motion to our findings, we also found that no C-mode was preferentially enriched with high-motion frames in all species (one-way ANOVA, p > 0.3, F < 1.14 for all comparisons, Fig. S5B).
fMRI C-modes exhibit evolutionarily conserved functional organization
Having identified dominant fMRI C-modes in all three species, we next asked whether their functional organization would show, on top of foreseeable species-specific features, recognizable evolutionarily conserved anatomic features. For this purpose, we matched C-mode topographies based on the similarity of mean fMRI coactivation profile across a set of evolutionarily conserved resting-state networks (RSNs)7,27–30 (Fig. 1C). The chosen networks include the default mode (DMN), visual (VIS), somatomotor (SMN), limbic (LIMB), ventral (VAT), and dorsal (DAT) attention, frontoparietal (FPN), as well as key subcortical nuclei of the thalamus (TH) and hippocampus (HCP) and were selected based on the notion that, across these three species, they encompass partly-conserved neuroanatomical substrates7,10. A key exception of note is the lack of established phylogenetic precursors of the VAT, DAT, and FPN in the rodent brain10. For this reason, these networks were not included in the coactivation profile in mouse data. For matching purposes, we ordered C-modes for each species in decreasing joint occurrence rates, and labeled them H1-H4 for humans, M1-M4 for macaques, and R1-R3 for rodents (mice). To allow spatial comparisons between species, C-mode maps were z-scored spatially, and the mean of the normalized activity of voxels within each RSN mask was computed to build its corresponding profile vector (Fig. 1D, C-modes numbered by decreasing occurrence rate). Vectors were first matched using the Hungarian Algorithm31 from human C-modes (organized in decreasing occurrence rate) to macaques, then from macaques to mice.
The corresponding results are depicted as the topological correlation between network coactivation profiles of human and macaque datasets (HNU and MSC, Fig. 1E, F, respectively), as well as between macaques and mice (Fig. 1G). Human-to-macaque matching gave consistent results for both HNU and MSC datasets, with the spatial topography of human C-modes H1-H4 being best aligned to corresponding macaque C-modes M3, M4, M2, and M1, respectively (see also Fig. 2A). Because the human HNU dataset included more subjects and was performed at daylight hours as animal scans, we describe our results hereafter for the HNU as main dataset and present a summary of the obtained MSC results as Supplementary Fig. (Figs. S6). Macaque to mouse matching linked C-modes M1 to R1, M2 to R2, and M4 to R3, respectively (Fig. 1G, 2A). The resulting overall cross-species matchings yielded an organization linking C-modes H1-M3, H2-M4-R3, H3-M2-R2, and H4-M1-R1. For brevity, we henceforth refer to these as C-mode 1, C-mode 2, C-mode 3, and C-mode 4, respectively. As human and macaque C-mode 1 was not preferentially matched to any mouse C-modes, we refer to mouse C-modes as 2, 3, and 4 for consistency with those mapped in higher species throughout the manuscript.
Fig. 2. C-mode topography in awake humans, macaques, and mice.
A Z-scored C-mode maps (Left) and corresponding normalized network coactivation profiles (Right, mean ± SD of voxels within the network mask). B C-mode occurrence rates (mean ± SEM), and between C-mode comparisons (Kruskal–Wallis test, FDR corrected, n = 30 subjects with 10 sessions, n = 8 animals with 2 sessions, and n = 44 animals with 1 session for humans, macaques, and mice respectively). In humans, data from male and female subjects are labeled separately. aDMN anterior default mode, pDMN posterior default mode, SMN somatomotor, VIS visual, DAT dorsal attention, VAT ventral attention, FPN frontoparietal, LIMB limbic, TH thalamus, HC hippocampus, BF basal forebrain, Cd/Pu caudate/putamen. P values: ns, p > 0.05, ****p < 0.0001. Source data are provided as a Source Data file.
Collectively, cross-species C-mode matching revealed four topographically related C-modes in humans and macaques, three of which (C-modes 2–4) were also represented in the rodent brain (Fig. 2A). In all species, C-modes exhibited rich spatial organization encompassing positive and negative peaks of fMRI activity that together delineated a set of stereotypic network coactivation profiles (Fig. 2A, right panels). Specifically, C-mode 1 encompassed peaks of fMRI activity in DMN, accompanied by below-baseline activity in somatomotor and ventral attention areas in both humans and macaques. C-mode 2 exhibited coactivation of DMN areas in anticorrelation with somatomotor, visual, and limbic networks in all three species. Interestingly, while in humans, the hippocampus had below-baseline fMRI activity, macaques and mice showed above-baseline fMRI activity in this region. C-mode 3 activity peaked in the somatomotor areas and concomitantly engaged most cortical regions of the human brain, albeit with considerably weaker or negative fMRI activity in visual, thalamic, and basal forebrain areas, reminiscent of global fMRI signal (GS) fluctuations. Finally, C-mode 4 was characterized in humans and macaques by positive coactivation in visual and posterior cortical regions, and negative coactivation in frontoparietal cortical regions. This topography in mice and macaques, but not humans, was associated with co-deactivation of the anterior cingulate and prefrontal regions of the DMN. Taken together, these results point to the presence of notable topographic correspondences in the functional organization of dominant C-modes in human, macaque, and mouse brains.
We next computed for each C-mode its occurrence rate, defined here as the proportion of fMRI frames assigned to each C-mode for each subject/animal. Interestingly, while the spatial organization of C-modes exhibited species-invariant topographic features, their occurrence rate, showed variation across species (Fig. 2B). Specifically, in humans, we observed a dominant occurrence of C-mode 1 and 2 (Kruskal–Wallis test, p < 0.001) while mice occurrence of sensory-oriented C-mode 3 and 4 was observed instead (Kruskal–Wallis test, p < 0.001), with macaques showing equiprobable (Kruskal–Wallis test, p = 0.25) C-mode occurrence (although a slight trend for a mouse-like profile was apparent). Thus, the temporal structure of C-modes in humans is a preferred occurrence of configurations led by associative cortical networks such as the DMN (C-modes 1 and 2), with mice and possibly macaques exhibiting instead greater occurrence of C-modes encompassing coactivation of sensory unimodal areas (C-modes 3 and 4). To further corroborate these results, we additionally computed the occurrence rates of individual CAPs (Fig. S7) and compared them with their corresponding anti-CAP (both of which compose a C-mode), finding no significant differences between these pairs of distributions in any of the analyzed datasets (p > 0.2, signed-rank test). This result supports the notion that C-modes reliably encapsulate key dynamic properties of their constituting CAPs.
fMRI C-modes exhibit infraslow fluctuations in humans, macaques, and mice
Our formalism allowed us to compare C-Modes with single fMRI frames and thus uncover the temporal structure of the spatial configurations that underlie spontaneous fMRI dynamics. We leveraged this property to describe, in all species, the temporal evolution of each C-mode for each subject/animal by computing the instantaneous spatial correlation between each C-mode map and each fMRI frame in all timeseries. The power spectra of the resulting C-mode timeseries revealed that C-modes undergo infraslow fluctuations in all species, with most of the power peaking within the 0.01–0.03 Hz range (Fig. 3). Power within the infraslow band was significantly greater than the mean power of surrogate timeseries generated from shuffling the joint C-mode timeseries 1000 times (Fig. 3, black traces). Peaks of infraslow activity were distinct and sharp in humans, and slightly less prominent (yet clearly recognizable) in mice and macaques.
Fig. 3. Infraslow dynamics and formation of C-modes.
Group-level power spectral density (blue, mean ± SEM) of C-Mode to fMRI frame correlation time-series. Red insets denote the mean ± SEM correlation values time-locked to peaks in the C-Mode time-series. Black traces show the spectrum of randomly shuffled surrogate C-mode time-courses. Source data are provided as a Source Data file.
We next investigated the assembly of each C-mode by normalizing (z-score) its time-course and by time locking peaks of C-mode time-courses (i.e., event-wavelet) at the group level. We found that C-modes in awake humans, macaques, and mice assemble and disassemble in a slow and gradual fashion (Fig. 3, red insets), reminiscent of damped oscillations in the dominant frequency band. These results suggest that fMRI C-modes exhibit comparable infraslow dynamic cycling in all the examined mammalian species.
fMRI C-modes occur at specific phases of fMRI global signal cycles in humans, macaques, and mice
Prompted by previous investigations in anesthetized mice16,29, we next probed if C-modes in higher species have a preferred occurrence within fMRI global signal (GS) cycles. We thus first ascertained that also in awake conditions, the GS would dominantly fluctuate within the infraslow range in all species (Fig. 4A). A spectral analysis showed that the power spectrum of GS sharply peaks within a 0.01–0.03 Hz band in awake humans, with analogous (albeit less pronounced) peaks of activity in the same frequency range in both awake macaques and mice. We then built a circular distribution of the phases of the filtered (0.01–0.03 Hz) GS at which each C-mode occurred, by sampling only occurrences in which the normalized C-mode timeseries surpassed threshold values of 1 SD. C-mode occurrence was significantly phase-locked within GS-cycles in humans, macaques, and mice (Fig. 4B, Rayleigh test, p < 0.05, FDR corrected). Interestingly, GS-phase distributions in macaques and humans presented key similarities, with C-modes 1, 3 and 4 (but not 2) exhibiting remarkably conserved cross-species phase alignment. Moreover, C-modes 3 and 4 showed broadly similar circular means across all the three species examined here.
Fig. 4. C-mode occurrence within fMRI global signal infraslow cycles.
A Group-level power spectral density (mean ± SEM) of the GS. B Distribution of GS-phases at the occurrence of each C-mode. Blue and red distributions correspond to GS-phases sampled from the positive and negative C-mode time-courses, respectively. All distributions significantly deviate from circular uniformity (Rayleigh test, one-sided, human, n = 30 subjects with ten sessions p < 0.0001 all C-modes, macaque n = 8 animals with two sessions p < 0.02 all C-modes; mouse, n = 44 animals with one session, p < 0.03 all C-modes; all p values are FDR corrected). Source data are provided as a Source Data file.
C-modes were computed by coalescing a CAP with an anti-CAP of nearly identical spatial shape but opposite fMRI coactivation. This description of brain dynamics works well under the assumption that CAP and anti-CAPS represent a single fluctuating brain sub-state whose pattern of activity changes sign cyclically. To corroborate the cyclic nature of C-mode fluctuations within GS-cycles, we repeated the sampling of GS-phases but with inverted (negative) C-mode time-courses so to capture the full temporal evolution of C-mode. This approach revealed clearly opposite distributions of sampling of the GS-phases from positive and negative C-mode occurrences (Fig. 4B, red insets). These results suggest that C-modes describe cycling spatiotemporal sub-state that fluctuates in magnitude and sign according to the infraslow structure of the global fMRI signal.
To further provide evidence of the infraslow oscillatory nature of C-mode dynamics, we investigated whether different C-modes were phase-coupled within individual GS-cycles. We computed the GS-phase difference between the occurrences of a C-mode in a GS-cycle, and the following occurrences of another C-mode either within the same GS-cycle, or in an immediately subsequent cycle (Fig. S8). These analyses showed highly consistent positioning of a C-mode within each GS-cycle (Fig. S8, diagonals) and phase coupling between some C-mode pairs (see C-mode 2 and 3 and 3 and 4 in humans, and C-modes 3 and 4 in mice). These results suggest that in mammalian species, intrinsic fMRI signal fluctuations do not reflect spatially undifferentiated peaks and troughs of fMRI activity, but instead encompass infralow cycling between dominant patterns of fMRI activity. This general principle can be extended to entail the evolutionary conservation of the phase relationship with fMRI GS-cycles for most (albeit not all, see C-mode 2) of the explored C-modes. In sum, these results show that C-modes are phase-locked to intrinsic GS fluctuations and imply that different C-modes can be conceptualized as networks of coupled oscillators in multiple mammalian species.
Coupled oscillatory activity explains C-mode transition dynamics
We next considered whether the temporal structure of C-mode instantaneous transitions could similarly be underpinned by species-invariant principles. For each species, we modeled the system as a Markov process from sequences of concatenated C-mode occurrences and computed the transition probability into a different C-mode, as well as C-mode self-transitions (also termed persistence probability, Fig. 5A). We found that the most recurring C-modes (1–2 in humans, 3–4 in macaques, and 3–4 in mice) were those with the highest probability of being transitioned to (p < 0.01, black crosses in Fig. 5A). Thus, they can be conceptualized as sinks of preferred directional transitions.
Fig. 5. Temporal trajectories of C-modes converge to the most recurring state.
A Persistence (top row) and transition (off-diagonal) probability in humans (left), macaques (middle), and mice (right). Black crosses denote the destination C-mode of preferred directional transitions (Pij > Pji). B Entropy of Markov trajectories (HMT) shows that the C-mode with the highest accessibility are C-modes with higher occurrence rates (i.e., C-mode 1 in humans and C-mode 4 in macaques and mice). Higher entropy indicates lower accessibility of a destination C-mode (column) from a starting C-mode(row). C Quantification of the sum of Entropy of Markov Trajectories for all destinations (columns in the HMT matrices at the single-subject level), and comparison between the means (one-way ANOVA, and Tukey test for multiple comparisons, n = 30 subjects with ten sessions, n = 8 animals with two sessions, and n = 44 animals with one session for humans, macaques, and mice, respectively). The most occurring C-modes (cf. Fig. 2) are also the most accessible ones. Student T-test, two-tailed, FDR corrected, *****p < 0.0001. In humans, data from male and female subjects are labeled separately. Source data are provided as a Source Data file.
Furthermore, we investigated, for each species, the accessibility of C-modes from one another by computing the corresponding Entropy of Markov Trajectories (HMT)32 from the transition probability matrices. This parameter measures the complexity of a transition: low entropy values imply an almost deterministic direct path or high accessibility. On the contrary, high entropy values suggest high uncertainty, requiring random steps through different C-modes before reaching the destination, i.e., low accessibility. In keeping with the C-mode occurrence rates we describe in Fig. 2B, the most recurring C-modes in all species were also those that were most accessible, i.e., they were the ones characterized by the lowest entropy values (Fig. 5B, C). Specifically, in humans, C-modes 1 and 2 were the most accessible ones (p < 0.0001 against C-modes 3–4). In macaques, we did not find any C-mode to have preferred accessibility (p = 0.25) Conversely, in mice, C-modes 3 and specially 4 were the most accessible ones (p < 0.0001). Importantly, in all species, the ensuing accessibility profile (Fig. 5C) recapitulated the C-mode occurrence rates we described in Fig. 2B, with the most accessible C-modes being also the most occurring ones (Fig. 2B). This was true also in macaques, where the entropy of Markov trajectories required to reach C-modes 3 and 4 followed the tendency of higher occurrence rates of these C-modes.
In the sections above, we characterized intrinsic fMRI dynamics both in terms of coupled quasi-periodic fluctuations between coactive networks captured by different C-modes, and also in terms of transitions between C-Modes. If coupled cyclic activity is key to explaining state transition dynamics, we expect that trajectories between C-modes with lower entropy (i.e., small HMT, corresponding to more direct transitions) would occur on average with shorter infraslow phase differences. Conversely, C-mode pairs with larger entropy (i.e., higher HMT corresponding to less direct transitions) would occur on average with longer infraslow phase differences. To test this hypothesis, we computed the circular-linear correlation between the mean GS-phase differences between C-mode occurrences (depicted in Fig. S8) and the HMT for their trajectories. We found clear and significant correlation values in all species (r = 0.66, 0.66, 0.87 with p = 0.029, 0.031, and 0.032 for humans, macaques, and mice, respectively). This implies that the transition structure of C-modes is, in part, described by infraslow-coupled cyclic dynamics across all species, embedding fast transition phenomena within GS-cycles in the dominant infraslow band.
fMRI C-mode occurrence predicts the ranking of connectivity gradients in humans, macaques, and mice
Previous investigations have shown that a high portion of the variance in static fMRI connectivity is explained by a limited fraction (5–15%) of fMRI frames exhibiting exceedingly high co-fluctuation amplitude16,33,34. We thus investigated whether the dynamic occurrence of dominant C-modes alone could similarly be sufficient to reconstitute key organizational features of the static fMRI connectome. To this aim, we first examined whether the occurrence-weighted average of C-modes would reproduce the static architecture of fMRI connectivity. To this purpose, we calculated for each C-modes its co-fluctuation matrix, i.e., the cross-multiplication of each C-mode map with itself 29. We found that in all species, the weighted average by occurrence rate of C-mode co-fluctuation matrices exhibited a high correlation with the corresponding group-mean static fMRI connectivity matrix (r > 0.57, all species, with r = 0.75 in humans, Fig. S9). These findings corroborate the notion that C-modes dynamics account for high co-fluctuation events critical for the topographic organization of the static functional connectome.
To further investigate the relationship between C-mode dynamics and static fMRI connectivity, we next inquired whether C-mode occurrence could also be related to the organization of fMRI connectivity gradients20,35. Gradient approaches were favored here because of their ability to chart the high-dimensional static functional connectome into low-dimensional manifold representations. In humans, the principal gradient of resting-state fMRI delineates a hierarchical organizational axis that reconstitutes the unimodal-transmodal spectrum in cortical regions. Crucially, this arrangement spatially recapitulates key myeloarchitectural, cytoarchitectural, and transcriptional features of the human and animal neocortex36,37. Higher-order gradients have similarly been linked to axes of differentiation between sensory cortical modalities. Here, we examined how the dynamic organization of fMRI activity relates to the spatial organization of gradients. We posited that C-mode occurrence rate may be linked to the ranking of functional connectivity gradients, with dominant (i.e., most occurring) C-modes aligning with the gradients that explain the most variation in fMRI connectivity. To test this hypothesis, we first computed for each species the top five fMRI connectivity gradients, which we next ranked by decreasing variance explained (Fig. S10). We then compared, for each species, the spatial similarity of the obtained gradients to that of each C-mode, matching them according to their highest absolute spatial correlation. Supporting our hypothesis, plausible spatial correspondences between C-mode and gradient topographies were observed in all species (Fig. 6A). Specifically, in humans, the most occurring C-modes 1 and 2 were matched with dominant gradients 1 and 2, while in macaques and mice, dominant gradients 1 and 2 were matched with most occurring C-modes 4 and 3. Moreover, in all species, C-mode occurrence was linearly related to the variance explained by each gradient (R2 > 0.78 for all species, Fig. 6B). This analysis shows how recurring network interaction represented by dominant C-modes, and their relative occurrence rate, shape the organization of the static fMRI connectome and its principal axis of variance in all the probed species.
Fig. 6. C-modes occurrence rate predicts ranking of functional connectivity gradients.
A Spatial correlation (r) between the principal gradients and each C-mode map. Black dots denote the spatial matching between maps obtained using the Hungarian Algorithm. B Scatter plot of the variance explained by each gradient (lambda) versus the corresponding C-mode occurrence rate. R-square from a linear fit (Pearson’s R, student T-test p < 0.01 uncorrected, two-sided, n = 30 subjects with ten sessions, n = 8 animals with two sessions, and n = 44 animals with one session for humans, macaques, and mice, respectively). The most occurring C-modes account for the most variance in the corresponding gradient axis. Source data are provided as a Source Data file.
Discussion
While previous cross-species studies have attempted to compare the static functional architecture of specific fMRI networks via time-invariant fMRI connectivity mapping10,38,39, whether the dynamic organization of intrinsic fMRI activity is evolutionarily conserved remains unclear. To fill this knowledge gap, we performed a systematic investigation of intrinsic fMRI dynamics in awake humans, macaques, and mice using the same analytical framework. To facilitate cross-species comparisons, we introduced a parsimonious description of network dynamics via fMRI C-modes, which represent dominant patterns of fluctuating fMRI activity. Using this simple approach, we found that fMRI dynamics in awake humans, macaques, and mice encompasses the recurring occurrence of a set of functionally related C-mode topographies. We also report that the dynamic structure of fMRI network activity follows a set of evolutionarily invariant principles. These include the observation that C-modes undergo infraslow fluctuations and tend to occur at specific phases of the fMRI global signal. Moreover, their transition structure is partly explained by infraslow coupled oscillator dynamics within fMRI global signal cycles. We also show that C-mode occurrence accounts for high co-fluctuation events critical to the topographic organization of the static functional connectome, and is closely related to the ranking of connectivity gradients. These observations suggest that, beyond and above some expected species-specific features, the dynamic structure of intrinsic fMRI activity in the awake mammalian brain follows evolutionarily conserved principles.
Although the lack of systematic comparisons of the dynamic organization of fMRI activity across species does not allow us to directly relate our findings to prior literature, our results are consistent with emerging evidence supporting the presence of cross-species homologies in the static organization of fMRI connectivity in the mammalian brain30,40. Our results expand these initial investigations, by showing that correspondences in static fMRI network organization can be parsimoniously described and accounted for by a limited set of evolutionarily-related dynamic patterns of fMRI activity. In keeping with this, the topographic organization of C-modes encompasses peaks of fMRI activity spanning network systems previously described in multiple mammalian species, including components of the DMN, salience, and motor-sensory networks, as well as in many key subcortical systems12,27,29. Extending previous observations29,41,42, we also found that C-modes dynamics can be reliably related to (and as such, it explains) the static organization of the fMRI connectome in all the probed species. These findings reconcile prior investigations of the dynamic structure of fMRI connectivity in rodents16,43–45, macaques46, and humans15,17,26,47–51 by showing that C-mode dynamics encompass high-amplitude peaks of fMRI activity that critically shape the steady-state architecture of the fMRI connectome. These results are also in agreement with the assumption that the mechanisms underlying inter-areal information transfer as assessed with fMRI are conserved in the mammalian brain52,53.
Our observation that the dynamic structure of fMRI activity in mammals follows species-invariant principles is important in light of the notion that many fundamental physiological and anatomical features (including the brain’s anatomical architecture), are known to scale with body size, or to be marginally (or not) represented in lower species54–56. Our data suggests instead that, independent of evolutionary complexity, infraslow fluctuations of spatially rich patterns of fMRI activity similarly underpin spontaneous brain activity in multiple mammalian species. In this respect, one important advancement of the present study is the use of datasets collected in awake conditions in all species, an experimental strategy that allowed us to carry out a cross-species comparison of fMRI dynamics unconfounded by the pharmacological effects (and the ensuing brain state changes) produced by anesthesia. An additional key benefit of the C-mode framework we introduce here is computational tractability, which allows for the representation of the corresponding spatiotemporal patterns of fMRI activity with voxel resolution (i.e., without predefined anatomical boundaries) and avoiding the use of correlation-based metrics (e.g., like in sliding window-based dynamic connectivity mapping). Moreover, the employed approach allows for a fine-grained temporal localization of dynamic effects with single-frame resolution. All of these properties were key to the identification of the dynamic properties and cross-species correspondences we report in this work.
The voxel-wise topography of C-modes enabled us to match and compare these spatiotemporal patterns at maximum spatial resolution, revealing a set of anatomically related motifs that exhibit evidence of evolutionary conservation across species. Functional correspondences between humans and macaques were apparent, and encompassed four neuroanatomically homologous C-modes that were plausibly identified in two independent human datasets. While evolutionarily more tentative, spatial matching between macaque and mouse was also plausible, with preserved fMRI polarity in key anchor networks in the three matched C-modes, and evidence of conserved C-mode GS-phase coupling in both species. Improved anatomical matching may be obtained in future studies by incorporating data from lissenchephalic new world monkeys, and other primate species phylogenetically closer to rodents that could serve as intermediate evolutionary link between macaques and mice8,57,58. This approach may represent a warranted extension of our work, owing to the increased availability of fMRI data in multiple primate species8.
The observation of a substantial coupling between C-mode occurrence and fMRI global signal cycling was first reported in anesthetized mice16 and it is here extended to awake humans and macaques. This finding corroborates the hypothesis that fluctuations in fMRI GS are the intrinsic manifestation of highly structured network interactions59. This result is also of interest in the light of emerging evidence linking global fMRI activity to intrinsic fluctuations in arousal26,60–62. Within this framework our findings suggests that the cycling spatiotemporal patterns of activity captured by C-modes (or by other analogous computational frameworks, like quasi-periodic patterns26,33), could be strongly affected or driven by ascending modulatory transmission.
Over the last decade, several influential mathematical models have described resting-state activity in terms of networks of coupled oscillators63–65. This work provides key empirical support to this modeling as it shows from data, and without making assumptions about the mechanism, that transition probabilities between different brain states can be described in terms of coupled C-Modes oscillating at infraslow frequencies. Moreover, the fact that the state transitions can be reliably described as coupled oscillators in all species suggests that a functional architecture based on coupled oscillating networks offers important evolutionarily advantageous computational benefits. These include the possibility of rapidly reconfiguring coordination and communication between different brain regions, and effectively transferring information across scales and brain areas21,53,66. Importantly, our work also aligns with the emerging hypothesis that oscillations are not generated at the local level, but represent slow frequencies of macroscopic modes of resonance in brain anatomy64,65.
While the present work focuses on the description of species-invariant principles underlying the organization of spontaneous fMRI activity, species-specific features were also apparent. Beyond foreseeable discrepancies in the topography of specific C-modes, which can be related to the increased complexity of the cortical mantle across the phylogenetic tree, one interesting difference we observed is a shift in C-mode occurrence across species. Although comparable human C-modes were identified in macaques and mice, their occurrence rate was inversed, with C-modes 1–2 being the most occurring patterns in humans and C-modes three to four in mice and in macaques. Taken together, these observations support the evolutionary basis for ongoing dynamic changes across species, albeit with a possible shift in the time that each species spends in each spatiotemporal state. Such species-dependent shifts in C-mode occurrence may reflect brain adaptations that support the increasing demands of higher-order cognition throughout evolution. The finding that in humans, fMRI activity profiles in C-modes 1–2 and 3–4 peak in polymodal and sensory areas, respectively, suggests that the intrinsic organization of fMRI activity is biased towards introspective states that are less frequently visited in animals. Accordingly, we found that these most recurrent C-modes are also the most easily accessible from any other C-mode.
Our results also highlight cross-species correspondences in the relationship between the dynamic organization of fMRI activity, and principal axes of variance of static fMRI activity as probed with connectivity gradients. We found that temporally dominant C-mode spatially reconstitutes the principal axis of static functional organization. This positive relationship—i.e., the occurrence of C-mode and the order of the functional gradient—is preserved across species, suggesting a fundamental conservation of how dynamic states underpin static functional organization across species. Interestingly, in previous studies, the transmodal-unimodal hierarchy gradient was commonly identified as the dominant organizational axis of static fMRI connectivity in humans35. However, this gradient has not been universally observed as the foremost gradient in other species35,37,67,68. By examining static functional organization through the lens of functional dynamics, our study provides a deeper understanding of this discrepancy, suggesting that it can be attributed to the different occurrences of underlying C-mode dynamics. Taken together, these results the macroscale organization of the functional connectome is critically shaped by the occurrence of its constituting spatiotemporal modes, a notion supported also by complementary conceptualization of fMRI dynamics65.
It should be noted that C-mode 3 exhibited a low correlation to its assigned gradient in both human and macaque datasets. This C-mode involves simultaneous coactivation of most brain regions and is temporally aligned with fMRI GS-cycles. As such, C-mode 3 reflects a state of global activity for which an axis of sorts is not the leading dimension explaining variance in the functional connectomes. In keeping with this, previous studies in mice69, macaques20, and humans35 did not identify an organizational axis in which most of the brain networks act in unison. We, therefore, did not expect to find a similar global gradient in either species. To maintain a semi-quantitative matching, we nonetheless allowed the algorithm to suggest which was the gradient most spatially similar to this C-mode. Future studies will warrant dedicated investigations of how periods of near whole-brain coactivation can be best accounted for as variance descriptors of static connectomes.
Because our fMRI timeseries were preprocessed and normalized, the polarity of our averaged fMRI signals cannot be assumed to directly reflect the positive or negative BOLD activity. Uncertainty as to the determinants of intrinsic BOLD activity remains, with recent evidence suggesting that it may be influenced also by non-neural signals, including cerebrospinal fluid pulsations70,71. Models of neurovascular coupling are still being validated to help interpret what BOLD activity implies at the neurovascular level72,73, and establishing a direct physiological link between peaks and throughs of fMRI activity in C-modes and the actual polarity of the underlying BOLD signal was beyond the scope of this investigation. We, however, corroborated the oscillatory nature of dominant large-scale network configurations in the mammalian brain, and showed how their dynamic features are species-invariant. Future studies employing concurrent widefield neural activity readouts and fMRI will be crucial to expand this interpretation.
In summary, we describe a conserved set of dynamic rules governing large-scale fMRI network dynamics in humans, macaques, and mice. Our work provides a simple and flexible framework to quantitatively model and relate intrinsic fMRI dynamics across the phylogenetic tree.
Methods
Data and preprocessing
Resting-state fMRI (rsfMRI) datasets from awake, freely breathing humans, macaques, and mice were used in this study. Human and macaque datasets were previously published (see references below). Preprocessing included most steps suggested by the guidelines of the Human Connectome Project74, using a combination of fMRI-dedicated software AFNI75, FSL76, FreeSurfer77, and SPM12 (http://fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/).
Human
The main dataset, the Hangzhou Normal University of the Consortium for Reliability and Reproducibility (CoRR-HNU, or HNU)22 includes 10 sessions of 10-min scans over the course of a month from n = 30 young healthy adults with no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders, head injuries, nor substance abuse (balanced sexes, age = 24 ± 2.41 years). Before scanning, participants were asked to relax and remain still with their eyes opened, avoiding falling asleep. During scanning, a black crosshair was shown in the middle of a gray background. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Center for Cognition and Brain Disorders at Hangzhou Normal University, and all participants signed written consent before data collection. A GE MR750 3T scanner (GE Medical Systems, Waukesha, WI, USA) was used to acquire MRI data. Functional scans were acquired with an echo-planar imaging sequence - EPI: TR = 2 s, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, FOV = 220 × 220 mm, matrix = 64 × 64, voxel-size = 3.4 mm isotropic, 43 slices. Data were downloaded from the International Neuroimaging Data-Sharing Initiative (INDI - http://fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.org/indi/CoRR/).
The Midnight Scan Club (MSC) dataset23 was used as a secondary, replication dataset. 5 out of 10 randomly selected sessions were used on separate days, and included 30-min scans from n = 10 healthy young adults (balanced sexes, age = 29.1 ± 3.3 years). Participants were asked to visually fix on a white crosshair against a black background. The study was approved by the Washington University School of Medicine Human Studies Committee and Institutional Review Board, and all participants signed written consent before scanning. Functional scans were acquired with a Siemens TRIO 3T MRI scanner (Erlangen, Germany) using a gradient-echo EPI sequence: TR = 2.2 s, TE = 27 ms, flip angle = 90°, voxel-size = 4 mm isotropic, 36 slices. Data were downloaded from OpenNeuro (10.18112/openneuro.ds000224.v1.0.3).
Preprocessing. The first 5 fMRI volumes were removed from each subject’s raw data, then despiking (AFNI 3dDespike) and slice-timing correction (AFNI 3dTshift) was performed. Data subsequently underwent motion-correction (AFNI 3dvolreg); skull-stripping (FSL fast and bet78); co-registration to the MNI 3 mm isotropic template (FSL flirt); regression of nuisance parameters (white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, and 24 motion parameters (6 parameters, 6 derivatives, and their respective squared time-series) (AFNI 3dDeconvolve); band-pass filtering between 0.01–0.1 Hz (AFNI 3dBandpass); spatial smoothing with a 6 mm FWHM kernel (AFNI 3dBlurInMask); and voxel time-series were finally normalized to z-scores (zero-mean, and standard deviation units).
Macaque
The Newcastle dataset (NC) includes n = 14 rhesus macaque monkeys (Macaca Mulatta) scanned with no contrast agents, from which N = 10 animals (two females, eight males), in which two independent fMRI sessions were available, were used for our analyses (age = 2.28 ± 2.33, weight = 11.76 ± 3.38). Two additional (female) animals were discarded from our analyses due to excessive head motion exceeding, in either session, over 30% of fMRI volumes with Frame-wise Displacement above a 0.3 mm threshold. Animal procedures, head-fixation, and protocols were approved by the UK Home Office and comply with the Animal Scientific Procedures Act (1986) on the care and use of animals in research and with the European Directive on the protection of animals used in research (2010/63/EU) (see ref. 24 for protocol specifics on animal preparation for awake imaging24). A Vertical Bruker 4.7T primate dedicated scanner was used, and rsfMRI experiments were performed in awake, head-fixed animals for two separate sessions with TR = 2 s; TE = 16 ms, voxel-size = 1.2 mm isotropic. Data were downloaded from NHP data-sharing consortium PRIME-DE (http://fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.org/indi/indiPRIME.html)25.
Preprocessing. The first 5 rsfMRI volumes were removed from each animal’s raw data, then despiking (AFNI 3dDespike) and slice-timing correction (AFNI 3dTshift) was performed. Data subsequently underwent motion-correction (AFNI 3dvolreg); skull-stripping (FSL fast and bet78); co-registration to the Yerkes19 2 mm isotropic template79 (FSL flirt); regression of nuisance parameters (white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, and 24 motion parameters (six parameters, six derivatives, and their respective squared time-series) (AFNI 3dDeconvolve); band-pass filtering between 0.01–0.1 Hz (AFNI 3dBandpass); spatial smoothing with a 3 mm FWHM kernel (AFNI 3dBlurInMask); and voxel time-series were finally normalized to z-scores (zero-mean, and standard deviation units).
Mouse
C57BL/6J mouse data were obtained from n = 44, head-fixed awake male mice undergoing a 12-min rsfMRI scan29. In vivo experiments were conducted in accordance with the Italian law (DL 26/214, EU 63/2010, Ministero della Sanita, Roma) and with the National Institute of Health recommendations for the care and use of laboratory animals. The animal research protocols for this study were reviewed and approved by the Italian Ministry of Health and the animal care committee of the University of Trento, and Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia (IIT). All surgeries were performed under anesthesia. Young adult (<12 months old) male C57BL/6 J mice were used. RsfMRI scans, both retrieved and newly acquired, were acquired at the IIT laboratory in Rovereto (Italy) using a Bruker 7T scanner (Bruker Biospin, Ettlingen) with a BGA-9 gradient set, 72 mm birdcage transmit coil, and a four-channel solenoid receiver coil: TR = 1 s, TE = 15 ms, flip angle = 60°, matrix = 100 × 100, FOV = 2.3 × 2.3 cm, 18 coronal slices 0.6 mm thick, 12 min total acquisition time.
Preprocessing. The first 120 rsfMRI volumes (2 min) were removed from each animal’s raw data to account for thermal gradient equilibration, then despiking (AFNI 3dDespike) was performed. Due to the short TR we did not perform slice-timing correction in these images. Data subsequently underwent motion-correction (FSL mcflirt); skull-stripping (FSL fast and bet78); co-registration to an in-house mouse brain template of 0.23 × 0.23 × 0.6 mm3 (ANTS registration suite80); regression of nuisance parameters (white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, and 24 motion parameters (6 parameters, 6 derivatives, and their respective squared time-series) (AFNI 3dDeconvolve); band-pass filtering between 0.01–0.1 Hz (AFNI 3dBandpass); spatial smoothing with a 0.5 mm FWHM kernel (AFNI 3dBlurInMask); and voxel time-series were finally normalized to z-scores (zero-mean, and standard deviation units).
Whole brain CAP detection and cluster-number selection
To identify recurrent rsfMRI whole-brain states, we used the whole-brain coactivation patterns (CAPs) approach15–17 in which fMRI frames are cluster based on their spatial similarity and then averaged to define recurrent patterns of fMRI coactivation. Specifically, for each species, we first performed censoring of motion-contaminated frames (frame-wise displacement: FD >0.3, 0.3, and 0.075 mm for humans2, macaques28, and mice29 respectively), and then concatenated the frames from all subjects or animals. Given that clustering human fMRI data in the 3 mm MNI-space became computationally challenging owing to its large dimensionality (n-voxels = 43,539, compared to 11,402 in macaques, and 8937 in mice), this step was carried out upon reducing data using the coarse 950-ROI Craddock Parcellation81. The choice of this specific parcellation regards its ability to provide a fair dimensionality reduction, while preserving information present at the voxel scale81. After these final steps, we ran, for each species, the k-means clustering algorithm16,82 (spatial correlation as the distance metric, 500 iterations, 5 replications with different random initializations, from k = 2:20, 5 independent runs). CAP maps were obtained at the group level by averaging the fMRI frames belonging to a cluster at the voxel level, then normalizing these values to T-scores from the concatenated datasets (Fig. S1). At the single-subject level, we obtained, for each subject/animal, a CAP map for each cluster by only averaging and converting to T-scores, the frames belonging to a cluster but only within a subject/animal’s data/session. We note that CAP mapping through frame averaging in humans was done at the voxel level, as the parcellated data was only used for clustering purposes. After clustering, we recovered the censored fMRI frames to the cropped datasets and assigned them to the CAP with the highest spatial correlation. This was done in order to have a continuum of frames for subsequent analyses.
Selection of the optimal number of clusters was done following a set of previously proposed empirical rules16,19, as well as newly proposed metrics. These were dependent on the availability of test-retest sessions within a dataset in macaques and humans, as well as a full independent dataset in humans from a different site. Specifically, for human HNU and MSC datasets independently, we first ran the k-means clustering algorithm with the concatenated dataset from k = 2:20, selecting, for each partition, the solution with the highest variance explained16 from five replications in five independent runs. Here, the variance of the data explained by each partition is defined as defined as the ratio between the between-cluster variance and the total variance (within-cluster + between-cluster variance). Within-cluster variance was computed as the averaged (over clusters) sum of square distances between elements in a cluster and its centroid. Between-cluster variance was computed as the averaged square distance between a cluster centroid and the centroid of all clusters or centroid of all data16,83. For each dataset independently, we first computed the variance explained by data partitioned into an increasing number of clusters (2 ≤ k ≤ 20) (Fig. S2A, B). We next assessed the topographic consistency of CAPs at increasing partitions. To this purpose, we computed how consistent CAPs are by assessing, for each k partition, the spatial correlation between a mean CAP map, and its matched map in the previous order (k-1) partition. Matching was done using the Hungarian Algorithm31. We found that partitions between k = 6:10 yielded, in both datasets, topographically stable CAPs that could be reliably identified at higher partitions (Fig. S2C, D).
We then assessed the within-dataset repeatability by comparing the spatial correlations between the mean CAP maps of each subject between each independent fMRI sessions (10 for HNU, and 5 for MSC, Fig. S2E, F). Statistical significance of the mean within-subject repeatability of each CAP was assessed by recomputing the spatial correlations between subject-level CAP maps after randomly shuffling the CAP-identity of fMRI frames, preserving occurrence rates. This process was repeated 1000 times, and repeatability values for each subject beneath the highest permutation value were flagged as non-repeatable (asterisks in Fig. S2E, F). The result of these comparisons showed that within an upper limit of k = 13 (HNU) or 15 (MSC), all the mapped CAPs were represented in all fMRI sessions of each subject, with significant spatial correlation across sessions (p < 0.05, surrogate testing with randomly shuffled cluster associations). At higher partitions, one or more CAPs were instead no longer represented in one or more subjects. We further probed the within-session stability of clustering by computing the CAP occurrence rate obtained across imaging sessions. We found that the CAP occurrence rate (i.e., the proportion of fMRI frames associated with a CAP in each subject), for both datasets, was stable across sessions at k = 2, 6, 8, and 9 (Kruskal–Wallis test, ten groups for HNU, five groups for MSC, FDR corrected for k comparisons, Fig. S2G, H).
Finally, to maximize the generalizability of our partitioning, we compared the main dataset’s (HNU) CAP maps with those obtained in the MSC in terms of topography matching and occurrence rates. To this aim, we spatially compared mean group-level CAP maps (matched with the Hungarian Algorithm for each partition) and tested the significance of this comparison by recomputing the values after randomly shuffling the fMRI frames within each dataset, while preserving occurrence rates (Fig. S2I). The mean occurrence rates from each dataset was assessed with a Wilcoxon signed-rank test, p < 0.05, FDR corrected for k comparisons (Fig. S2J). This analysis revealed that clusters from all partitions, except k = 6, 7, 9, 14, 15, and 16, were topographically reproducible across datasets (Fig. S2I). By contrast, a comparison of the mean CAP occurrence rates between datasets (Fig. S2I) showed that k = 8 was the only partition in which this parameter was conserved across datasets (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, p < 0.05, FDR corrected for k comparisons). Based on these analyses, k = 8 was the only partition level meeting all the required within and between subject/dataset reproducibility criteria. We thus based all our subsequent analyses of human fMRI dynamics using k = 8 clusters.
Selection of optimal clusters in macaque fMRI time-series (Newcastle test-retest datasets, n = 8 animals, two sessions) followed the same strategy employed for human time-series. Computation of explained variance in macaque fMRI time-series revealed an elbow region within the k = 6:10 range (Fig. S3A). Macaque CAPs were topographically stable at increasing k partitions until an upper limit at k = 10 (Fig. S3B). Comparing the repeatability of CAP topographies across sessions revealed that above k = 8, some CAPs present a lack of topographical reproducibility (p > 0.05, surrogate testing with randomly shuffled cluster associations), as well as significant differences in CAP occurrence rates (Kruskal–Wallis test, FDR corrected for k comparisons, Fig. S3C, D). This cumulative evidence suggests that k = 8 was the highest partition that guarantees CAP stability, as well as test-retest topographical and frame distribution repeatability amongst clusters.
For mice, the variance explained curve for the awake dataset we used in this work (N = 44) showed an elbow between 6 and 8 clusters (Fig. S3E, F). Topographic CAP stability as a function of increasing partition number revealed that mouse CAPs were topographically stable up to k = 6. This value is in agreement with the results of CAP number selection in prior independent studies29,39, in which k = 6 was consistently identified as the optimal partition in this species.
Coactivation modes and between-species matching
In previous work we demonstrated that CAPs appear in pairs of spatial configurations with opposite fMRI coactivations16 (CAP, and anti-CAP, Fig. 1A). As this feature was initially detected in awake and anesthetized mice29, we first showed that these anatomical configurations were present also in all of our datasets. For each species, we thus organized CAPs in descending occurrence rate and computed the Pearson’s correlation between the vectorized mean CAP maps as a topological similarity metric, matching CAPs and anti-CAPs as pairs with the highest anticorrelation coefficient (Fig. 1B). We then reduced pairs into C-modes, by taking each CAP map pair (T-score normalized), subtracting the least recurring one from its counterpart, then dividing the resulting map by two (Fig. 1A). In order to have a normalized basis for between-species comparisons, we further z-scored each resulting C-mode map. Given that C-modes represent unique configurations between known resting-state networks16,17,19, we extracted the mean fMRI values from voxels within masks of evolutionarily conserved30,53 cortical RSN in humans27, macaques28 and mice29,69, as well as hippocampal and thalamic masks74,84 (Fig. 1C). To best match DMN organization across species, we split this network into an anterior and a posterior component. This choice was made to account for recent primate and mouse evidence of rostrocaudal disassembly of this network in some datasets and species29,58. In rodents, this is especially apparent in awake conditions, where parietal and peri-hippocampal cortical components that are synchronously connected to the mouse PFC (in anesthesia) tend to form dissociable networks in awake animals29. For consistency with previous studies, we included the caudate-putamen in the mouse DMN (but not in primate and human DMN) as in rodents, this region is proximal and highly synchronous with anterior cingulate hubs of the DMN both under anesthesia and in awake conditions12. Pilot studies showed that the inclusion (or lack thereof) of the CPu in mouse DMN did not alter monkey-to-human C-Mode matching.
For each C-mode, we computed their occurrence rates as the average occurrence of the CAP and anti-CAP conforming to the C-mode. We then organized the vectorized network coactivation profiles of each C-mode in descending occurrence rate order, and matched C-mode profiles of humans (C-mode H1-4) to macaques (C-mode M1-4), then macaques to mice (C-mode R1-3, rodent) using the Hungarian Algorithm31. This matching was conducted using vector-correlations between profiles as distance metrics for the cost function to be minimized (Fig. 1D). We represented the similarities between network coactivation profiles of C-modes in the human HNU dataset and macaques (Fig. 1E), as well as with the MSC dataset and macaques, confirming our matching (Fig. 1F). Finally, we matched macaque C-mode network coactivation profiles with those of mice, without accounting for frontoparietal (FPN), ventral (VAT) and Dorsal Attention (DAT) networks, as these have not been described in the mouse brain (Fig. 1G). We note that since C-mode 1 was not reliably identified in mice, most likely due to the lack of weighting by higher-order VAT, DAT, and FPN, we hereafter exclude C-mode 1 from this species.
Because C-modes fundamentally include an anticorrelated counterpart, in pilot studies, we probed cross-species matching using absolute value correlations as opposed to positive correlation only. We found that only macaque M4 and mouse R3, as well as M1 and the inverse of R1, produced a different match when absolute value correlation was used as distance metrics. Matching between human and macaque C-modes remained unchanged. However, while this alternative matching appeared to be spatially plausible, only matching using positive spatial correlation as a cost function resulted in a plausible cross-species preservation of phase coupling with fMRI global signal cycles in macaques and mouse. For this reason, we retained positive correlation-based matching as a reference dataset for the present manuscript, as we believe it provides a more plausible representation of key evolutionarily relevant dynamic features of C-modes.
After matching and organizing C-modes, we mapped the voxel-wise C-modes as well as the network coactivation profiles within C-modes (within-network voxel mean ± SD, Fig. 2A). Independent group-level CAP maps for each human (HNU and MSC); macaque and mouse dataset are shown in Fig. S4). For each subject/animal in each dataset, we computed a C-mode’s occurrence rate as the proportion of fMRI frames associated with the CAP pairs composing the C-mode. A comparison of the distributions of occurrence rates was then performed (Kruskal–Wallis test, FDR corrected).
We further tested that in-scanner head motion did not selectively affect the configuration of any C-mode. This was done by first independently clustering the non-motion censored rsfMRI datasets with k = 8, 8, and 6 for humans, macaques, and mice, respectively; building C-modes; and then comparing the topographies of matched C-mode maps (Hungarian Algorithm31) to the ones obtained after censoring (Pearson’s Correlation) (Fig. S5A). We then computed, for each C-mode in each subject/animal, the proportion of frames in it that were classified as high-head motion (FD >0.3, 0.3, and 0.075 mm for humans, macaques, and mice, respectively), and compared these distributions across C-modes (one-way ANOVA, Fig. S5B).
C-mode infraslow dynamics, formation, and temporal structure
We investigated the temporal evolution of C-modes by generating a time-course of instantaneous C-mode to fMRI frame spatial correlation (hereafter referred to as C-mode time-courses) for each subject/animal, and then computing the power spectrum of these time-courses as well as that of the Global fMRI signal (GS, i.e., the average of all voxels at each frame). We next computed the group mean ± SEM power spectra. C-mode time-courses were normalized to standard deviation units and mean zero. The significance of the observed infraslow portion of C-mode spectra was assessed by calculating the power spectrum of surrogate C-mode timeseries obtained after jointly shuffling the timepoints of the original C-mode time-courses using the same random sequence of time points. This method preserves the signal amplitude distribution and the correlation structure of the original timeseries16. We next carried out, for each frequency, a one-tailed two-sample t-test (FDR corrected for comparisons within the 0.01–0.1 Hz band) comparing the power of the original C-mode PSD with the mean of 1000 iterations of the surrogates. The evolution of the formation of a C-mode was computed by time-lock averaging the C-mode to frame correlation values at the vicinity of a peak surpassing 1 SD (±30 s), then averaging these event-kernels at the group level (Fig. 3 insets).
Given that the GS also presented a strong peak in the power spectrum in the [0.01–0.03 Hz] infraslow band (Fig. 4A), we computed whether the occurrence of C-modes was phase-locked to infraslow GS fluctuations16,29. We extracted the instantaneous infraslow phase of the GS (filtered between 0.01–0.03 Hz) using the Hilbert Transform, then divided the trace into cycles of minimum 30 s and maximum 100 s. For each cycle, if a C-mode was present, we sampled the GS-phase at which the C-mode occurred, retaining only samples from normalized C-mode time-courses exceeding 1 SD, thus guaranteeing the selection of frames that are reasonably well assigned to a specific C-mode16. We then built the distribution of GS-phases from the group-concatenated sampling of C-mode occurrences, and using MATLAB’s CircStats toolbox85, we performed a Rayleigh test (p < 0.001, FDR corrected) for deviations from circular uniformity (Fig. 4B). To confirm the cyclical features of C-mode fluctuations within GS-cycles, we repeated this analysis but sampling GS-phases from occurrences of inverted (negative) C-mode time-courses (Fig. 4B, red insets). As in our previous work16, we also demonstrated that C-modes are phase-locked within GS-cycles by sampling the GS-phase difference between the occurrence of C-modes within a GS-cycle, and the following occurrences of another C-mode within the same cycle or the immediately subsequent one, limiting sampling to instances in which C-mode time-courses exceeded 1 standard deviation (Fig. S8).
To further explore the temporal sequencing of C-mode occurrences and transitions (Fig. 5), we defined, for each dataset, a concatenated sequence of C-mode occurrences across subjects/animals, and calculated the transition probability matrix from a C-mode i at time t to another C-mode j at time as the proportion of transitions and all other transitions from i19,29. Only transitions within the same subject were included, and we first considered sequences in which we counted the auto-transitions (), namely persistence probabilities. Off-diagonal elements (), named transition probabilities, were then computed after building sequences in which we removed the repeating elements in order to control for auto-correlations given the C-mode’s dwell time19,86. We quantified the directional prevalence of a transition () by taking their difference. To measure the relative facilitation to reach a C-mode from another, we computed the Entropy of Markov Trajectories (HMT)29,32 from the off-diagonal matrix elements (i.e. transition probabilities). This method calculates the descriptive complexity of the paths between C-mode (in bits), where a lower complexity refers to less information required to access a destination C-mode j from a source C-mode i, hence being more accessible as the path travels through less path before reaching its destination. Specifically, for a Markov Chain (MC) defined by transition probability matrix P, we define the Entropy Rate per step as:
1 |
Where is the stationary distribution solving Also define the Entropy of a Markov Trajectory from C-mode i to C-mode j as:
2 |
Where , M is a matrix of stationary probabilities ; I the identity matrix; is the matrix of single-step entropies (from C-mode i to any C-mode k); and is a diagonal entropy matrix with trajectories from a C-mode to itself , with zeros if 32.
Statistical significance of persistence probabilities was tested from random sequences by generating 1000 permutations of C-mode occurrence sequences at the subject/animal level, concatenating the sequences, and counting how many times a random iteration exceeded the true value to reach a p value. Transition probabilities and directional transition prevalence were tested against matrices built after randomly permuting the non-repeating sequences 1000 times at the subject/animal level, concatenating them and removing samples if . These surrogates were also used to test the significance of Markov trajectory entropies. To further quantify and assess the relative complexity of transitions to a particular destination C-mode, we quantified, after repeating the analyses with subject level sequences, the sum of the HMT matrix columns, and compared their distributions across C-mode destinations (one-way ANOVA and Tukey test for multiple comparisons, Fig. 5C). Finally, to assess the relationship of the mean GS-phase difference between occurrences of different C-modes and the HMT of their transitions, we computed the circular-linear correlations of their mean values (Fig. 5B and S8, respectively) using the circ_corrcl.m Matlab function85.
C-mode influence on static fMRI connectome and functional connectivity gradients
We first generated C-mode co-fluctuation matrices by cross-multiplying the mean C-mode map with itself42, which yielded a voxel-wise representation of the co-activating (or co-deactivating) peaks of fMRI activity. We then computed the group-level mean Functional Connectivity matrices by computing the Pearson’s Correlation between the concatenated time-series of voxels in all subjects/animals in each group87, and computed the correlation between the vectorized upper triangular part of the FC matrix to the weighted (by occurrence rate) average of the C-mode Co-fluctuation matrices29 (Fig. S9A, B). Voxels were ordered according to the RSN they belong to (see Fig. 1C).
We further explored if the hierarchies that dominate FC are in accordance with the dynamic structure of C-modes. We first computed the gradients of FC matrices from each species using the Diffusion Mapping method (BrainSpace88 - diffusion_mapping.m Matlab function: FC matrix sparse to top 10-percentile per node, 5 components, anisotropic diffusion parameter = 0.5)69,89. We mapped the gradients (Figure S10) and computed their spatial correlation to the obtained C-modes, matching each C-mode to the gradient with the highest absolute Pearson’s correlation (Fig. 6A). Finally, we plotted the variance explained by each gradient (lambda) to the group mean occurrence rate of their matched C-mode, fitting a linear model to this relationship (Fig. 6B).
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Source data
Acknowledgements
This work has received funding from the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (#DISCONN; no. 802371 to A.G.). The authors also acknowledge funding from the Simons Foundation (SFARI 982347 to A.G. and S.P).
Author contributions
A.G. and D.G.-B. conceived the study. D.G.-B., A.G., and T.X. curated and preprocessed the data, and D.G.B. developed the computational analyses with input from A.G., T.X., and S.P. A.G. and D.G.-B. wrote the manuscript with input from T.X., S.P., and J.S.B.R.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Communications thanks Joana Cabral and Sung-Ho Lee for their contribution to the peer review of this work. A peer review file is available.
Data availability
All raw rsfMRI data can be found in the following repositories: Human—HNU55: http://fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.org/indi/CoRR/. Human – MSC56: https://openneuro.org/datasets/ds000224/versions/1.0.3/download. Macaque – NC59: http://fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.org/indi/PRIME/newcastle.html. Mouse rsfMRI raw data generated in this study has been deposited in a Mendeley Data repository: Gutierrez-Barragan, Daniel (2024), “awake_rsfMRI_mouse_NatComms2024”, Mendeley Data, V2, 10.17632/vtk54hzzfb.2. Processed data underlying the results in this study is available upon request from the authors. Source data are provided with this paper.
Code availability
Scripts to reproduce our analyses can be found in the following repository: https://github.com/danielgb87/cmodes.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors jointly supervised this work: Ting Xu, Alessandro Gozzi
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41467-024-52721-8.
References
- 1.Smith, S. M. et al. Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA106, 13040–13045 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Power, J. D. et al. Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage84, 320–341 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Craddock, R. C. et al. Imaging human connectomes at the macroscale. Nat. Methods10, 524–539 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Preti, M. G., Bolton, T. A. & Van De Ville, D. The dynamic functional connectome: State-of-the-art and perspectives. Neuroimage160, 41–54 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Lurie, D. J. et al. Questions and controversies in the study of time-varying functional connectivity in resting fMRI. Netw. Neurosci.4, 30–69 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gozzi, A. & Zerbi, V. Modeling brain dysconnectivity in rodents. Biol. Psychiatry93, 419–429 (2023). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Pagani, M., Gutierrez‐Barragan, D., de Guzman, A. E., Xu, T. & Gozzi, A. Mapping and comparing fMRI connectivity networks across species. Commun. Biol.6, 1–15 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Milham, M. et al. Accelerating the evolution of nonhuman primate neuroimaging. Neuron105, 600–603 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Grandjean, J. et al. A consensus protocol for functional connectivity analysis in the rat brain. Nat. Neurosci.26, 673–681 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 10.Gozzi, A. & Schwarz, A. J. Large-scale functional connectivity networks in the rodent brain. Neuroimage127, 496–509 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Tsai, P. J. et al. Converging structural and functional evidence for a rat salience network. Biol. Psychiatry88, 867–878 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Whitesell, J. D. et al. Regional, layer, and cell-type-specific connectivity of the mouse default mode network. Neuron109, 545–559.e8 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Pezzulo, G., Zorzi, M. & Corbetta, M. The secret life of predictive brains: what’s spontaneous activity for? Trends Cogn. Sci.25, 730–743 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Palva, J. M. & Palva, S. Infra-slow fluctuations in electrophysiological recordings, blood-oxygenation-level-dependent signals, and psychophysical time series. Neuroimage62, 2201–2211 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Karahanoğlu, F. I. & Van De Ville, D. Transient brain activity disentangles fMRI resting-state dynamics in terms of spatially and temporally overlapping networks. Nat. Commun.6, 7751 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gutierrez-Barragan, D., Basson, M. A. A., Panzeri, S. & Gozzi, A. Infraslow state fluctuations govern spontaneous fMRI network dynamics. Curr. Biol.29, 2295–2306.e5 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Liu, X., Chang, C. & Duyn, J. H. Decomposition of spontaneous brain activity into distinct fMRI co-activation patterns. Front. Syst. Neurosci.7, 1–11 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Cornblath, E. J. et al. Temporal sequences of brain activity at rest are constrained by white matter structure and modulated by cognitive demands. Commun. Biol.3, 261 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Huang, Z., Zhang, J., Wu, J., Mashour, G. A. & Hudetz, A. G. Temporal circuit of macroscale dynamic brain activity supports human consciousness. Sci. Adv.6, 1–15 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Xu, T. et al. Cross-species functional alignment reveals evolutionary hierarchy within the connectome. Neuroimage223, 117346 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Deco, G., Jirs, V., McIntosh, A. R., Sporns, O. & Kötter, R. Key role of coupling, delay, and noise in resting brain fluctuations. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA106, 10302–10307 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Zuo, X. N. et al. An open science resource for establishing reliability and reproducibility in functional connectomics. Sci. Data1, 1–13 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Gordon, E. M. et al. Precision functional mapping of individual human brains. Neuron95, 791–807.e7 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Slater, H. et al. Individually customisable non-invasive head immobilisation system for non-human primates with an option for voluntary engagement. J. Neurosci. Methods269, 46–60 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Milham, M. P. et al. An open resource for non-human primate imaging. Neuron100, 61–74.e2 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bolt, T. et al. A parsimonious description of global functional brain organization in three spatiotemporal patterns. Nat. Neurosci.25, 1093–1103 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Thomas Yeo, B. T. et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol.106, 1125–1165 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Xu, T. et al. Interindividual variability of functional connectivity in awake and anesthetized rhesus macaque monkeys. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging4, 543–553 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Gutierrez-Barragan, D. et al. Unique spatiotemporal fMRI dynamics in the awake mouse brain. Curr. Biol.32, 631–644.e6 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Mars, R. B. et al. Whole brain comparative anatomy using connectivity blueprints. Elife7, 1–15 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kuhn, H. W. The Hungarian method for the assignment problem. Nav. Res. Logist.52, 7–21 (2005). [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ekroot, L. & Cover, T. M. The entropy of Markov trajectories. IEEE Trans. Inf. Theory39, 1418–1421 (1993). [Google Scholar]
- 33.Esfahlani, F. Z. et al. High-amplitude cofluctuations in cortical activity drive functional connectivity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA117, 28393–28401 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ragone, E. et al. Modular subgraphs in large-scale connectomes underpin spontaneous co-fluctuation events in mouse and human brains. Commun. Biol.7, 126 (2024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 35.Margulies, D. S. et al. Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA113, 12574–12579 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hong, S. J. et al. Toward a connectivity gradient-based framework for reproducible biomarker discovery. Neuroimage223, 117322 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Huntenburg, J. M., Bazin, P. L. & Margulies, D. S. Large-scale gradients in human cortical organization. Trends Cogn. Sci.22, 21–31 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Lu, H. et al. Rat brains also have a default mode network. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA109, 3979–3984 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Mandino, F. et al. A triple-network organization for the mouse brain. Mol. Psychiatry27, 865–872 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Pagani, M., Gutierrez-Barragan, D., De Guzman, E. & Gozzi, A. In Advances in Resting-State Functional MRI (Elsevier, 2023).
- 41.Abbas, A. et al. Quasi-periodic patterns contribute to functional connectivity in the brain. Neuroimage191, 193–204 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Faskowitz, J., Esfahlani, F. Z., Jo, Y., Sporns, O. & Betzel, R. F. Edge-centric functional network representations of human cerebral cortex reveal overlapping system-level architecture. Nat. Neurosci.23, 1644–1654 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Belloy, M. E. et al. Resting brain fluctuations are intrinsically coupled to visual response Dynamics. Cereb. Cortex31, 1511–1522 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Grandjean, J. et al. Dynamic reorganization of intrinsic functional networks in the mouse brain. Neuroimage152, 497–508 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Ma, Y., Hamilton, C. & Zhang, N. Dynamic connectivity patterns in conscious and unconscious Brain. Brain Connect7, 1–12 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Barttfeld, P. et al. Signature of consciousness in the dynamics of resting-state brain activity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA112, 887–892 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Allen, E. A. et al. Tracking whole-brain connectivity dynamics in the resting state. Cereb. Cortex24, 663–676 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Cabral, J. et al. Cognitive performance in healthy older adults relates to spontaneous switching between states of functional connectivity during rest. Sci. Rep.7, 1–13 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Demertzi, A. et al. Human consciousness is supported by dynamic complex patterns of brain signal coordination. Sci. Adv.5, eaat7603 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Huang, Z., Zhang, J., Wu, J., Mashour, G. A. & Hudetz, A. G. Temporal circuit of macroscale dynamic brain activity supports human consciousness. Sci. Adv.6, 1–10 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Sporns, O., Faskowitz, J., Teixeira, A. S., Cutts, S. A. & Betzel, R. F. Dynamic expression of brain functional systems disclosed by fine-scale analysis of edge time series. Netw. Neurosci.5, 405–433 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Buckner, R. L. & DiNicola, L. M. The brain’s default network: updated anatomy, physiology and evolving insights. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.20, 593–608 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Griffa, A. et al. Evidence for increased parallel information transmission in human brain networks compared to macaques and male mice. Nat. Commun.14, 8216 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Zhang, K. & Sejnowski, T. J. A universal scaling law between gray matter and white matter of cerebral cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA97, 5621–5626 (2000). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Ardesch, D. J. et al. Scaling principles of white matter connectivity in the human and nonhuman primate brain. Cereb. Cortex32, 2831–2842 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Lindstedt, S. L. & Schaeffer, P. J. Use of allometry in predicting anatomical and physiological parameters of mammals. Lab. Anim.36, 1–19 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Buckner, R. L. & Krienen, F. M. The evolution of distributed association networks in the human brain. Trends Cogn. Sci.17, 648–665 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Garin, C. M. et al. An evolutionary gap in primate default mode network organization. Cell Rep.39, 110669 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Ville, D. VanDe Brain dynamics: global pulse and brain state switching. Curr. Biol.29, R690–R692 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Liu, X. et al. Subcortical evidence for a contribution of arousal to fMRI studies of brain activity. Nat. Commun.9, 1–10 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Raut, R. V. et al. Global waves synchronize the brain’s functional systems with fluctuating arousal. Sci. Adv.7, 1–16 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Shine, J. M. et al. The dynamics of functional brain networks: integrated network states during cognitive task performance. Neuron92, 544–554 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Ponce-Alvarez, A. et al. Resting-state temporal synchronization networks emerge from connectivity topology and heterogeneity. PLoS Comput. Biol.11, 1–23 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Pang, J. C. et al. Geometric constraints on human brain function. Nature618, 566–574 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Cabral, J., Fernandes, F. F. & Shemesh, N. Intrinsic macroscale oscillatory modes driving long range functional connectivity in female rat brains detected by ultrafast fMRI. Nat. Commun.14, 375 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Deco, G., Tononi, G., Boly, M. & Kringelbach, M. L. Rethinking segregation and integration: contributions of whole-brain modelling. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.16, 430–439 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Katsumi, Y. et al. Correspondence of functional connectivity gradients across human isocortex, cerebellum, and hippocampus. Commun. Biol.6, 401 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Bethlehem, R. A. I. et al. Dispersion of functional gradients across the adult lifespan. Neuroimage222, 117299 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Coletta, L. et al. Network structure of the mouse brain connectome with voxel resolution. Sci. Adv.6, 1–43 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Fultz, N. E. et al. Coupled electrophysiological, hemodynamic, and cerebrospinal fluid oscillations in human sleep. Science366, 628–631 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Chen, J. E. et al. Resting-state “physiological networks. Neuroimage213, 116707 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Mateo, C., Knutsen, P. M., Tsai, P. S., Shih, A. Y. & Kleinfeld, D. Entrainment of arteriole vasomotor fluctuations by neural activity is a basis of blood-oxygenation-level-dependent “resting-State” bonnectivity. Neuron96, 936–948.e3 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Cerri, D. H. et al. Distinct neurochemical influences on fMRI response polarity in the striatum. Nat. Commun. 15, 1916 (2024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 74.Glasser, M. F. et al. The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage80, 105–124 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Cox, R. W. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res.29, 162–173 (1996). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Woolrich, M. W. & Smith, S. M. FSL - Review. Neuroimage62, 782–790 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Fischl, B. FreeSurfer. Neuroimage62, 774–781 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Smith, S. M. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp.17, 143–155 (2002). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Donahue, C. J. et al. Using diffusion tractography to predict cortical connection strength and distance: a quantitative comparison with tracers in the monkey. J. Neurosci.36, 6758–6770 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Tustison, N. J. et al. Large-scale evaluation of ANTs and FreeSurfer cortical thickness measurements. Neuroimage99, 166–179 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Craddock, R. C., James, G. A., Holtzheimer, P. E., Hu, X. P. & Mayberg, H. S. A whole brain fMRI atlas generated via spatially constrained spectral clustering. Hum. Brain Mapp.33, 1914–1928 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Arthur, D. & Vassilvitskii, S. K-Means++: the advantages of careful seeding. In Proc. eighteenth Annu. ACM-SIAM Symp. Discret. algorithms 1027–1025 (2007).
- 83.Goutte, C., Toft, P., Rostrup, E., Nielsen, F. Å. & Hansen, L. K. On clustering fMRI time series. Neuroimage9, 298–310 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Hartig, R. et al. The subcortical atlas of the rhesus macaque (SARM) for neuroimaging. Neuroimage235, 117996 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 85.Berens, P. CircStat: a MATLAB toolbox for circular statistics. J. Stat. Softw.31, 293–295 (2009). [Google Scholar]
- 86.Cornblath, E. J. et al. Temporal sequences of brain activity at rest are constrained by white matter structure and modulated by cognitive demands. Commun. Biol.3, 1–12 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Cho, J. W., Korchmaros, A., Vogelstein, J. T., Milham, M. P. & Xu, T. Impact of concatenating fMRI data on reliability for functional connectomics. Neuroimage226, 117549 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Vos de Wael, R. et al. BrainSpace: a toolbox for the analysis of macroscale gradients in neuroimaging and connectomics datasets. Commun. Biol. 3, 103 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 89.Ren, J. et al. Individual variability in functional organization of the human and monkey auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex31, 2450–2465 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All raw rsfMRI data can be found in the following repositories: Human—HNU55: http://fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.org/indi/CoRR/. Human – MSC56: https://openneuro.org/datasets/ds000224/versions/1.0.3/download. Macaque – NC59: http://fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.org/indi/PRIME/newcastle.html. Mouse rsfMRI raw data generated in this study has been deposited in a Mendeley Data repository: Gutierrez-Barragan, Daniel (2024), “awake_rsfMRI_mouse_NatComms2024”, Mendeley Data, V2, 10.17632/vtk54hzzfb.2. Processed data underlying the results in this study is available upon request from the authors. Source data are provided with this paper.
Scripts to reproduce our analyses can be found in the following repository: https://github.com/danielgb87/cmodes.